Effects of Cognitive Demand Levels for Various Horticultural Activities on Psychophysiological Responses in Adults

Authors:
Yun-Jin Kim Department of Bio and Healing Convergence, Graduate School, Konkuk University, Seoul, 05029, Republic of Korea

Search for other papers by Yun-Jin Kim in
This Site
Google Scholar
Close
and
Sin-Ae Park Department of Bio and Healing Convergence, Graduate School, Konkuk University, Seoul 05029, Republic of Korea, and Department of Systems Biotechnology, Konkuk Institute of Technology, Konkuk University, Seoul 05029, Republic of Korea

Search for other papers by Sin-Ae Park in
This Site
Google Scholar
Close

Click on author name to view affiliation information

Abstract

We compared the effects of horticultural activities according to cognitive demand levels on psychophysiological responses in adults. Thirty-two adults in their 20s were included. Participants performed 10 horticultural activities (raking, planting transplants, fertilizing, tying plants to stakes, harvesting, sowing, conducting cuttage, planting potted plants, cutting and washing, arranging flowers) for 150 seconds at two levels of cognitive demand. Electroencephalographic (EEG) and electrocardiographic measurements were acquired during the activity. After each activity, the participants’ emotional states were evaluated using the semantic differential method (SDM). The EEG results, according to comparison by activity, showed that for nine activities, excluding raking, relative theta decreased when performing tasks at a level of high cognitive difficulty (HCD) compared with those with a low cognitive difficulty (LCD), and relative beta, relative gamma, fast alpha, and relative low beta increased, indicating activation of the prefrontal cortex. In the relative theta power spectra, the cuttage activity was found to be the lowest when performing tasks at a high level high cognitive difficulty, and the working memory function was activated the most compared with other activities. When sowing at a low level of cognitive demand, participants’ heart rate decreased and stabilized. When potted plants were harvested at a high level of cognitive difficulty, the ratio of low frequency to high frequency increased, and the sympathetic nervous system was activated. In addition, when planting transplants, and cutting and washing were performed at a high level of cognitive difficulty, and the standard deviation of the RR interval was high, indicating a high ability of the autonomic nervous system to resist stress. As a result of the SDM, the emotional state according to task difficulty was found to be more stable and relaxed than high cognitive difficulty, but a significant increase in comfort, relaxation, and naturalness was achieved when nine gardening tasks with low cognitive difficulty were performed, with the exception of sowing. The results of this study show that tasks with high cognitive difficulty activate working memory, whereas those with low cognitive difficulty stabilize and relax brain activation. Therefore, this study confirmed that an intervention in horticultural activities with an appropriate level of cognitive difficulty could have a significant effect on psychophysiological changes in adults.

Urbanization in modern society has forced more than half of the world’s population to live in urban areas (Ohly et al. 2016), leading to a disconnect between humans and nature. This reduced contact with the natural environment exposes humans to various environmental stressors (Stilgoe 2001). According to the attention restoration theory, modern urban lifestyles increase demands on cognitive resources, and intensified demands induce a state of attentional fatigue (Kaplan 1995; Kaplan and Berman 2010). In contrast, contact with nature has a positive effect on overall well-being, including a positive impact on psychological and mental health (Chiesura 2004; Hung and Chang 2021). In addition, reconnection between humans and nature can lead to the recovery of cognitive functions, such as memory, attention, and concentration, and can increase sustainability (Ives et al. 2018; Lee et al. 2021).

In modern life, horticulture is a representative activity in which people interact easily and intimately with nature. It includes a variety of plant-based activities, such as planting plants, arranging flowers, and appreciating natural landscapes, and it provides opportunities to interact with living organisms, such as animals (Chen et al. 2013). Horticultural therapy is an activity that improves or maintains the functions of the human mind and body through plants or horticultural activities and is used as a nondrug interventional treatment. Horticultural therapy induces curiosity and improves judgment and coping skills. The repetition of these experiences leads to increased knowledge and skills training, thus improving self-esteem (Kang and Kang 2021). In addition, by observing plants, creativity and self-expression can be developed to show emotional effects (Kang and Kang 2021; Whear et al. 2014). By providing opportunities for social interaction, students can learn how to cooperate, divide labor, and communicate effectively (Whear et al. 2014). Interventional horticultural treatment using plant activities provides health benefits in terms of physical, psychological, emotional, and cognitive aspects, and can have a positive impact on quality of life (Park et al. 2016, 2017, 2020).

Many studies have been conducted to demonstrate the effects of horticultural activities and therapies. In terms of physical aspects, energy consumption was measured to determine the exercise intensity of gardening activities (Park et al. 2011, 2013, 2014), and muscle activity was measured to investigate the muscles activated according to gardening activities (Park et al. 2013, 2014). In addition, motion analysis was conducted to identify kinematic factors, such as joint angles and gripping patterns, according to gardening activities (Lee et al. 2016, 2018). Regarding psychoemotional aspects, brain waves were measured by electroencephalography during gardening activities, and it was found that attentional concentration improved (Kim et al. 2020, 2021). When examining plants using near-infrared spectroscopy, the concentration of oxygen-hemoglobin was found to be reduced significantly, which had a positive effect on psychological relaxation (Oh et al. 2019; Park et al. 2017). In addition, there is a difference in the degree of interest and emotional change according to the type of flower when arranging flowers (Wu et al. 2022). In terms of cognition, brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels increased significantly after attending gardening programs and performing gardening activities, suggesting there is potential for cognitive improvement (Park et al. 2020). In addition, when soil mixing was performed at a high level of cognitive demand, the activity in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) increased significantly (Kim et al. 2022). Studies on the physical and psychoemotional aspects of horticultural activities are being actively conducted, whereas studies on the cognitive aspects are insufficient. Therefore, we investigated the effects of various horticultural activities according to cognitive demand levels by extending the study by Kim et al. (2022).

Cognitive load refers to the amount of cognitive demand required for working memory when performing mental tasks such as learning or problem solving (Mazher et al. 2017; Sweller 1994). Effective learning can occur when cognitive demands match working memory (Rebsamen et al. 2011). Working memory is a brain function that stores and provides temporarily information necessary for cognitive tasks such as language comprehension, learning, and reasoning (Backs and Walrath 1992), but there are limitations in capacity and time when storing or processing information (Fuentes-Garcia et al. 2019; Sweller 2011). Therefore, when the amount of information to be processed exceeds the amount of information that can be processed, the accessibility of working memory decreases, and the cognitive load increases, resulting in cognitive overload (Mazher et al. 2017; Paas et al. 2003). Thus, the working memory structure and its limitations must be considered when performing cognitive working memory assessments (Mazher et al. 2017).

Cognitive load theory considers cognitive overload to be a major cause of poor learning (Sweller 1994). The purpose of this study is to identify the causes of unnecessary cognitive load, induce effective learning, and develop learning strategies (Sweller 1994). To achieve effective learning, an appropriate level of cognition is required according to the subject. To identify this, it is necessary to measure the degree of cognitive load during the learning process. Measuring cognitive load helps to maintain an optimal cognitive load in a variety of environments and while conducting tasks with a level of high cognitive demand (Faller et al. 2019).

To identify the appropriate level of cognitive demand, it is important to measure and classify accurately the cognitive load required for working memory to identify an appropriate level of cognitive demand (Paas et al. 2003). There are various methods for measuring cognitive load. Previous studies have used methods such as heart rate variability (HRV) (Mazher et al. 2017), brain waves (electroencephalography) (Antonenko et al. 2010), skin conductance (Niedermeyer and da Silva 2005), and pupil changes (Gale and Edwards 1983) as physiological methods to measure cognitive load. Among these, electroencephalography has been used traditionally to study brain cognitive processes (Zarjam et al. 2011) and is a suitable tool for measuring the degree of cognitive load continuously (Chandler and Sweller 1991). In our study, electroencephalography and electrocardiography were measured, and the semantic differential method (SDM) was used to investigate their effects on the psychophysiological response and emotional state of adults in terms of cognitive aspects when performing various horticultural activities according to the level of cognitive demand.

Materials and Methods

Research participants

This study included 32 adults in their 20s (16 men and 16 women; average age, 23.2 ± 3.0 years). Participants were recruited using convenience sampling. Leaflets containing research information were distributed to schools, apartments, and libraries in Gwangjin-gu, Seoul, for recruitment. The inclusion criteria were as follows: no physical disability, no history of mental illness, no use of drugs that could affect cognitive ability, and right-hand dominance. Participants were asked to fast for 3 h before the experiment to prevent substances such as caffeine from stimulating brain activity and affecting the data (Heckman et al. 2010). Before conducting the study, the participants were informed of the contents of the study and precautions, and they then provided written consent. Age, gender, height, weight, and body mass index (ioi 353; Jawon Medical Co., Ltd., Gyeongsan, Korea) were recorded to collect demographic information. Participants visited the greenhouse three times during the experiment. After the experiment was completed, an incentive (∼US $50) was received. This study was approved by the Bioethics Committee of Konkuk University (7001355-202203-HR-519, March 2022).

Experimental conditions

This study was conducted in a greenhouse at Konkuk University in Seoul, Korea. Indoor horticulture activities were conducted inside a greenhouse (200 × 160 cm) (Fig. 1A). The environmental conditions of the experimental space were an average temperature of 24.3 ± 2.5 °C (O-257; DRETEC Co., Ltd., Saitama, Japan), with an average relative humidity of 23.7 ± 10.3% (O-257; DRETEC Co., Ltd.). The average illuminance was 9846.7 ± 2683.6 lux (ST-126; SINCON, Bucheon, Korea). Outdoor horticultural activities were carried out on a vegetable garden bed installed outside the greenhouse and were conducted in a space dedicated to the experiments (500 × 200 cm) (Fig. 1B). The environmental conditions of the experimental space included an average temperature of 20.6 ± 3.3 °C (O-257; DRETEC Co., Ltd.), an average relative humidity of 19.8 ± 6.3% (O-257; DRETEC Co., Ltd.), and an average illuminance of 18,717.5 ± 9191.6 lux (ST-126, SINCON).

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.

Experimental space layout. (A) Indoor experimental space. (B) Outdoor experimental space.

Citation: HortScience 58, 11; 10.21273/HORTSCI17272-23

Experimental procedure

Each participant followed the experimental protocol shown in Fig. 2. In this study, 10 types of horticultural activities were performed, and each horticultural activity was divided into two cognitive demand levels. Tasks with low cognitive difficulty consisted of simple activities with relatively few steps, whereas tasks performed at level of high cognitive difficulty consisted of complex activities with many steps. Three to four horticultural activities were performed per visit. Before starting the activity, each participant rested for 120 s. The participants then performed each horticultural activity for 150 s at two different cognitive demand levels. All participants received the same instructions and performed the activities in the same manner. The order of activities was assigned randomly to each participant.

Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.

Study procedure at each visit. EEG = electroencephalogram or electroencephalographic; ECG = electrocardiogram or electrocardiographic.

Citation: HortScience 58, 11; 10.21273/HORTSCI17272-23

The 10 horticultural activities used in this study were selected through consultation with five experts active in the field of horticultural therapy, and a manual was prepared to enable the tasks to be performed within the activity time (Table 1). The 10 horticultural activities were divided into indoor and outdoor activities. Indoor horticulture consisted of five activities: sowing, performing cuttage, planting potted plants, cutting and washing, and arranging flowers. Outdoor horticulture consisted of five activities: raking, planting transplants, fertilizing, tying plants to stakes, and harvesting. The selected horticultural activities are used frequently in horticultural therapy and gardening. Each task presented in the horticulture activity manual involved different levels of cognitive demand.

Table 1.

Horticulture activity manual.

Table 1.

When raking at a low level of cognitive difficulty, one grasps the rake, lifts it to head height, sticks it into the ground, and plows. When performed at a higher cognitive demand, in addition to the previous steps, one finds foreign objects and collects them. Planting transplants at a low level of cognitive difficulty entails digging the ground, planting seedlings such as lettuce, and covering them soil. At a higher level of cognitive demand, planting intervals are considered, locations are determined, and various seedlings are planted. Fertilizing at a low level of cognitive demand involves picking up solid fertilizer and spraying it around plants; at a higher level of cognitive demand one prepares and sprays liquid fertilizer onto the leaves. Tying plants to stakes at a low level of cognitive difficulty involves holding and fixing; at a high level of cognitive demand one must insert a support pole into the ground, then tie plants to it using a specific method. Harvesting lettuce leaves from a vegetable garden box involves a low cognitive demand; at a high level, specific lettuce varieties are chosen. Sowing at a low cognitive demand includes making holes, planting seeds, and covering them. At a high level of cognitive difficulty, lettuce seeds are prepared, absorbed, and placed carefully. Cuttage at a low level of cognitive demand entails separating and inserting cuttings; at high level, tree branches are pruned, leaves are trimmed, and growth promoter is applied. Planting potted plants at a low level of cognitive difficulty requires one to prepare pots and transfer plants; at a high level, additional steps such as determining soil type and isolating plants are added. The cutting and washing activity involves washing lettuce leaves at a low level of cognitive demand; at a high level, one must also remove wilted leaves and stems, and arrange them by size. Arranging flowers involves inserting trimmed flowers into foam (low cognitive demand), whereas at a high level of cognitive difficulty, flowers are prepared further, woven, and arranged creatively. These activities highlight the nuanced skills and considerations involved in different facets of horticultural practice.

Electroencephalography of the PFC was conducted during each activity. Then, after performing the activity for 150 s, the participants’ emotional responses were evaluated using the SDM. During the experiment, the participants were instructed not to make any noise or speak. All experimental procedures were performed over three visits, and each visit took an average of 70 min.

Measurement items

Physiological measurement.

Electroencephalography and electrocardiography were used for the physiological measurements. Electroencephalography is a noninvasive method for recording electrophysiological signals generated in the human brain (Lina and Karwowski 2020) and is widely used in the field of neuroscience (e.g., to evaluate motor function, cognitive load, attention level, and brain disorders) (Maddirala and Veluvolu 2021). In addition, it has been used traditionally to study cognitive processes (Fuentes-Garcia et al. 2019) and is a suitable tool for measuring the degree of cognitive load continuously (Antonenko et al. 2010). In our study, participants’ brain activity and HRV were measured using a wireless electroencephalographic (EEG) measuring device (Quick-20; Cognionics, San Diego, CA, USA) and medical electrodes (HP-OP42; Hurev, Wonju, Korea), in relation to the cognitive difficulty of the gardening activities. The EEG measurement device used in this study minimized the risk of electric shock using a dry electrode system.

We performed EEG monitoring on the left and right prefrontal lobes according to the international 10–20 electrode array system (Jasper 1958; Klem et al. 1999) (Fig. 3A). Data were collected by amplifying the electrical signals measured by contacting the scalp with dry electrodes. The EEG signals were collected at a sampling rate of 1 kHz. A reference electrode was placed on the left ear clip (Fig. 3B). The electrocardiographic (ECG) electrode was placed at the end of the clavicle, and the ground electrode was attached to the left rib area (Fig. 3B). This device has been certified by the European Commission and the Federal Communications Commission (Kim et al. 2020).

Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.

International electrode arrangement (Stevens et al. 2016). (A) The channels in bold type, shaded in blue (Fp1 and Fp2) were measured in this study. (B) Electrode placement in electrocardiography. G = ground electrode; M = measuring electrode.

Citation: HortScience 58, 11; 10.21273/HORTSCI17272-23

Psychological measurement.

The SDM was used as a physiological measure (Osgood et al. 1957) to examine participants’ psychological responses to the cognitive difficulties of gardening activities. The scale is a 13-point Likert scale that consists of three options: comfortable/uncomfortable, relaxed/awake, and natural/artificial. The higher the value of each item, the more positive the emotional state.

Data processing and analysis

The measured EEG data were analyzed using the Bio-scan analysis program (Bio-Tech, Daejeon, Korea). Data were collected at a speed of 1 kHz (bandpass, 0.5–100 Hz) using a brain mapping program (Bioteck Analysis Software, Daejeon) through the built-in amplifier. The collected EEG data were analyzed for five power spectra: relative theta (RT), relative beta (RB), relative gamma (RG), relative low beta (RLB), and relative fast alpha (RFA). Each indicator was analyzed according to the following criteria (Kim et al. 2022; Klink et al. 2020). The RT power spectrum was calculated and analyzed using the ratio of power in the 4- to 50-Hz band to power in the 4- to 8-Hz band, revealing cognitive and working memory status. Similarly, the RB power spectrum was calculated and analyzed using the ratio of power in the 4- to 50-Hz band to power in the 13- to 30-Hz band, indicating attentive status. The RG power spectrum was calculated and analyzed using the ratio of power in the 4- to 50-Hz band to power in the 30- to 50-Hz band, revealing cognitive and attentive status. For the RFA power spectrum, the ratio of power in the 4- to 50-Hz band to the power in the 11- to 13-Hz band was calculated and analyzed, demonstrating relaxation and stabilization. Last, the RLB power spectrum was calculated and analyzed using the ratio of power in the 4- to 50-Hz band to power in the 12- to 15-Hz band, indicating attentive status. These parameters represent the physiological state of the brain.

Results

Demographic characteristics

Adults age 23.2 ± 3.0 years participated in the study (16 men, 24.9 ± 3.3 years; 16 women, 21.6 ± 1.5 years) (Table 2). The average height was 166.4 ± 7.9 cm. The average body weight was 61.8 ± 10.0 kg. The overall average body mass index was 22.4 ± 3.4 kg⋅m–2, which is within the normal range per the criteria specified by the World Health Organization.

Table 2.

Study participant demographics.

Table 2.

Electroencephalography

Comparison by activity.

EEG analysis of 10 horticultural activities according to cognitive difficulties showed that cognition and attention were activated in the same frequency band as RT and RG according to the cognitive difficulties of nine horticultural activities, with the exception of raking (P < 0.05) (Table 3). Planting, and cutting and washing resulted in increased RFA and RLB markers in the bilateral PFC when performing tasks at a high level of cognitive difficulty (P < 0.05). With regard to the fertilizing activity, RG was found to increase in the PFC when performing tasks with a low level of high cognitive difficulty (P < 0.01), whereas RFA and RLB indicators were found to decrease (P < 0.05). Tying plants to stakes showed that RG increased in the left PFC when performing tasks with high cognitive difficulty (P < 0.01), and the RLB index decreased in the bilateral PFC (P < 0.05). Harvesting showed that the RLB index increased in the left PFC when performing tasks with a high level of cognitive difficulty (P < 0.05). Sowing found that RB and RG indicators increased in the PFC when performing tasks at a level high of cognitive difficulty (P < 0.01), whereas RT indicators decreased (P < 0.01). With regard to performing cuttage, RB, RG, and RFA indicators increased (P < 0.05), and RT indicators decreased (P < 0.001) in the bilateral PFC when performing tasks at a high level of cognitive difficulty. In planting potted plants, RFA and RLB indices increased in the bilateral PFC when performing tasks at a high level of cognitive difficulty (P < 0.05), and RG indices decreased in the left PFC (P < 0.05). With regard to arranging flowers, when performing tasks at a high level of cognitive difficulty, the RB index increased in the bilateral PFC (P < 0.01) and the RT index decreased in the left PFC (P < 0.01). In terms of raking, there were no significant differences according to level of cognitive difficulty (P > 0.05).

Table 3.

Encephalographic results according to activity (N = 32).

Table 3.

Comparison of activities.

Significant differences were found between the activities in the RT power spectra of both prefrontal lobes (P > 0.001) (Table 4). In the bilateral PFC, when cuttage was performed at a high cognitive difficulty, the RT was the lowest compared with the other activities. When fertilizing, sowing, performing cuttage, and arranging flowers were performed at a low level of cognitive difficulty, the RT was higher than for other activities.

Table 4.

Results of the relative theta power spectrai by electroencephalography according to activity (N = 32).

Table 4.

Electrocardiography

The ECG results showed a tendency to relax and stabilize when performing activities at a low level of cognitive difficulty compared with high cognitive difficulty. The heart rate during sowing decreased significantly for low-cognitive difficulty activities (P < 0.001) (Table 5). When harvesting and planting potted plants at a high level of cognitive difficulty, the low frequency increased and the high frequency decreased (P < 0.05). The standard deviation of NN intervals (SDNN) increased significantly when planting transplants, and cutting and washing at a high level of cognitive difficulty (P < 0.05). No significant differences were found in raking, fertilizing, tying plants to stakes, performing cuttage, and arranging flowers (P > 0.05).

Table 5.

Results of heart rate variability according to activity (N = 32).

Table 5.

Semantic differential method

The SDM results for activity difficulty are shown in Table 6. In terms of comfort, there was a significant increase in the activities of tying plants to stakes, harvesting, performing cuttage, cutting and washing, and arranging flowers at a low level of cognitive difficulty (P < 0.05). Regarding the relaxed aspect, it increased significantly when raking, planting transplants, harvesting, performing cuttage, planting potted plants, and cutting and washing were performed at a low level of cognitive difficulty (P < 0.05). In terms of the natural aspect, there was a significant increase when fertilizing, harvesting, performing cuttage, planting potted plants, cutting and washing, and arranging flowers were performed at a low level of cognitive difficulty (P < 0.05). No significant differences were found for sowing (P > 0.05). In nine horticultural activities, except sowing, when the cognitive demand for the activity was low, there was a more positive effect on the emotional aspect than when the cognitive demand for the activity is high.

Table 6.

Results of the semantic differential method according to activity (N = 32).

Table 6.

Discussion

This study investigated the psychophysiological responses of 32 adults according to the level of cognitive demand required for various horticultural activities. EEG and ECG measurements of the PFC were acquired to determine changes according to the level of cognitive demand required for gardening activities, and emotional states were compared using the SDM. EEG results were compared for the nine activities, excluding raking. When tasks were performed at a high level of cognitive difficulty, compared with the low level, RT decreased, and RB, RG, RFA, and RLB increased, indicating activation of the PFC (P < 0.05) (Table 3). The EEG results showed that RT was lowest when performing cuttage at a high level of cognitive difficulty, and it was high when fertilizing, sowing, performing cuttage, and arranging flowers were performed at a low level (P < 0.001) (Table 4). The ECG results showed that the sympathetic nervous system was activated and the parasympathetic nervous system was suppressed when performing activities with high cognitive demands (P < 0.05) (Table 5). The SDM showed that when performing nine activities at the low level of cognitive difficulty (the exception was sowing), a more positive mood state was felt than when performing these tasks at a high level of cognitive difficulty (P < 0.05) (Table 6).

The frontal lobe is located in the anterior part of the cerebral hemisphere; it controls higher mental processes and plays a role in coordinating information coming from other associations of the body and controlling behavior (Pope et al. 2019). It also plays an important role in various cognitive processes, such as attention, memory, and language (Chayer and Freedman 2001). In particular, the front part of the frontal lobe is called the PFC, and it plays an important role in cognitive functions, such as binding information to create a balanced system and executing planned contents (Siddiqui et al. 2008).

Brain waves occur naturally in both the active and resting states, and human thoughts, emotions, and behaviors reflect neural activity inside the brain (Sowndhararajan et al. 2015). Electroencephalography can relate different frequencies to brain activity by measuring the electrophysiological signals that occur in the brain (García-Monge et al. 2020). They are generally divided into delta (0–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–13 Hz), beta (13–30 Hz), and gamma (30–50 Hz) bands, according to the frequency bands that represent specific functional roles (Staufenbiel et al. 2014). In our study, the five indices RT, RB, RG, RFA, and RLB were used as parameters.

The RFA index increased when planting transplants, performing cuttage, potting plants, and cutting and washing at a high level of cognitive difficulty (P < 0.05) (Table 3). The RFA index is activated primarily when emotional anxiety is stabilized (Park 2005). Increased fast alpha waves can awaken the brain to a comfortable resting state and improve cognitive abilities (Kim et al. 2022). When sowing, performing cuttage, and arranging flowers were done at a high level of cognitive difficulty, the RT index decreased and the RB index increased in the PFC. In addition, the RLB index increased when planting transplants, harvesting, potting plants, and cutting and washing activities were performed at high cognitive difficulty. The RT band plays an important role in cognitive processing, memory, and learning mechanisms (García-Monge et al. 2020). In particular, theta and upper alpha waves play important roles in building working memory and long-term memory (Karakaş 2020). A reduction in theta waves indicated that cognitive function was activated. In addition, the RB band increases when attention is focused, such as when paying attention, solving problems, and making decisions (Neuper and Pfurtscheller 2001). Beta waves are classified into low, mid, and high beta waves. Their activation is related to academic performance, and they can help improve cognitive function (Sowndhararajan et al. 2015). The RG index increased when fertilizing, tying plants to stakes, sowing, and performing cuttage were performed at high cognitive difficulty. The RG band is related to perceptual coupling, such as attention, arousal, and learning (Fries 2015; Wang 2010), and occurs particularly in conjunction with various cognitive processes (Herrmann et al. 2004). This indicates that horticultural activities with high cognitive difficulty activate cognitive function by awakening the PFC.

When performing cuttage was performed at a high level of cognitive difficulty, the RT index appeared to be the lowest, indicating that working memory function was activated the most compared with other activities (P < 0.001) (Table 4). In addition, when the fertilizing, sowing, performing cuttage, and arranging flowers were performed at low cognitive difficulty, the RT index was high, indicating they required the least cognitive load. This indicates there are differences in the levels of cognitive difficulty associated with horticultural activities.

HRV reflects the interaction between the sympathetic nervous system and the parasympathetic nervous system of the autonomic nervous system (Kabisch et al. 2021). Changes in heart rate, the ratio of low frequency to high frequency, and SDNN were evaluated over time according to the time domain analysis method (Park and Jeong 2014). The sympathetic nervous system dominates under conditions of increased activity and stress, and HRV increases when stimulated (Gidlow et al. 2016; Pham et al. 2021; Shaffer and Ginsberg 2017). The parasympathetic nervous system predominates in quiet and relaxed states, and is associated with decreased HRV (Pham et al. 2021). In our study, when sowing at a low level of cognitive difficulty, the heart rate was lowered and stabilized (P < 0.05) (Table 5). The ratio of low frequency to high frequency increased when harvesting and planting potted plants were performed at a high level of cognitive difficulty (P < 0.05). In addition, the SDNN, which represents the standard deviation of the NN intervals between R-peaks of the heartbeat, increased when planting transplants and cutting and washing were performed at a high level of cognitive difficulty (P < 0.05). This means that when performing an activity at high cognitive difficulty, a greater stress level is experienced than when performing an activity at a low level of cognitive difficulty.

As a result of measuring the emotional states of people performing gardening activities according to cognitive difficulty using the SDM, significant differences were found in nine horticultural activities (P < 0.05; sowing was the exception) (Table 6). There is a significant difference in psychological emotions when the cognitive demand for the activity is high and when the cognitive demand for the activity is low in nine horticultural activities (sowing excluded). According to a previous study (Kim et al. 2022), there was no significant difference in cognitive demand when performing a soil-mixing activity, and this affected the activity of the frontal cortex without affecting the subjective psychological state. However, the results of our study show that there are activities that are affected by the emotional state according to level of cognitive difficulty and type of gardening activity.

In previous studies, the effects of horticultural activities on cognitive function have been identified, and the effects of single horticultural activities have been measured according to the level of cognitive demand used to complete them. In our study, various horticultural activities were divided into two levels of cognitive demand, and the effects on participants’ PFC activity and emotional state were investigated. We found that performing tasks at a high level of cognitive difficulty was more effective in activating cognitive functions and increasing attention than activities completed at a low level of cognitive difficulty. In addition, activities done at a low level of cognitive difficulty have a more positive effect on emotional stability than those performed at a high level. However, because the participants in this study were limited to adults in their 20s, it is difficult to generalize the results to all age groups. Thus, additional research is needed to expand this study and investigate the effects of various horticultural activities and levels of cognitive demand on participants’ cognitive function according to age group. Moreover, no significant difference in EEG results was observed with the raking activity. This suggests that the level of difficulty used to perform this activity was not extreme enough to effect a difference.

We measured the effects of various horticultural activities, performed at two levels of cognitive demand, on the psychophysiological responses of adults. With most of the 10 horticultural activities, the higher the level of cognitive demand required to perform the task, the greater the activity in the PFC, which activates cognitive function and attention. Depending on the level of cognitive demand, there is a difference in the amount of cognitive load required from participants. These results show the need for an intervention with an appropriate cognitive level of difficulty for the subject according to the purpose of horticultural activity (relaxation, training, etc.). The results of this study are believed to influence the control of cognitive function stimulation and fatigue through the intervention of an appropriate level of cognitive demand. In addition, this study provides basic data for the development of professional horticultural therapy treatment programs based on scientific evidence tailored to the subject.

References Cited

  • Antonenko P, Paas F, Grabner R, Van Gog T. 2010. Using electroencephalography to measure cognitive load. Educ Psychol Rev. 22:425438. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-010-9130-y.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Backs RW, Walrath LC. 1992. Eye movement and pupillary response indices of mental workload during visual search of symbolic displays. Appl Ergon. 23(4):243254. https://doi.org/10.1016/0003-6870(92)90152-L.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chandler P, Sweller J. 1991. Cognitive load theory and the format of instruction. Cogn Instr. 8:293332. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci0804_2.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chayer C, Freedman M. 2001. Frontal lobe functions. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 1:547552. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11910-001-0060-4.

  • Chen HM, Tu HM, Ho CI. 2013. Understanding biophilia leisure as facilitating well-being and the environment: An examination of participants’ attitudes toward horticultural activity. Leis Sci. 35:301319. https://doi.org/10.1080/01490400.2013.797323.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chiesura A. 2004. The role of urban parks for the sustainable city. Landsc Urban Plan. 68:129138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.landurbplan.2003.08.003.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Faller J, Cummings J, Saproo S, Sajda P. 2019. Regulation of arousal via online neurofeedback improves human performance in a demanding sensory-motor task. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 116:64826490. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1817207116.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fries P. 2015. Rhythms for cognition: Communication through coherence. Neuron. 88:220235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.034.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fuentes-Garcia JP, Pereira T, Castro MA, Carvalho Santos A, Villafaina S. 2019. Psychophysiological stress response of adolescent chess players during problem-solving tasks. Physiol Behav. 209:112609. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112609.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gale A, Edwards J. 1983. The EEG and human behavior. Physiological correlates of human behavior, p 99–127.

  • García-Monge A, Rodríguez-Navarro H, González-Calvo G, Bores-García D. 2020. Brain activity during different throwing games: EEG exploratory study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 17:6796. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17186796.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gidlow CJ, Jones MV, Hurst G, Masterson D, Clark-Carter D, Tarvainen MP, Smith G, Nieuwenhuijsen M. 2016. Where to put your best foot forward: Psycho-physiological responses to walking in natural and urban environments. J Environ Psychol. 45:2229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.11.003.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Heckman MA, Weil J, de Mejia EG. 2010. Caffeine (1,3,7-trimethylxanthine) in foods: A comprehensive review on consumption, functionality, safety, and regulatory matters. J Food Sci. 75:R77R87. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-3841.2010.01561.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Herrmann CS, Munk MH, Engel AK. 2004. Cognitive functions of gamma-band activity: Memory match and utilization. Trends Cogn Sci. 8(8):347355. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.06.006.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hung SH, Chang CY. 2021. Health benefits of evidence-based biophilic-designed environments: A review. J People Plants Environ. 24:116. https://doi.org/10.11628/ksppe.2021.24.1.1.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ives CD, Abson DJ, Wehrden HV, Dorninger C, Klaniecki K, Fischer J. 2018. Reconnecting with nature for sustainability. Sustain Sci. 13:13891397. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11625-018-0542-9.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jasper HH. 1958. The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 10:371375.

  • Kabisch N, Püffel C, Masztalerz O, Hemmerling J, Kraemer R. 2021. Physiological and psychological effects of visits to different urban green and street environments in older people: A field experiment in a dense inner-city area. Landsc Urban Plan. 207:103998. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.landurbplan.2020.103998.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kang KJ, Kang MJ. 2021. Effects of horticultural therapy on Korean elderly with dementia: A meta-analysis. J Korean Acad Psychiatr Ment Health Nurs. 30(4):352368. https://doi.org/10.12934/jkpmhn.2021.30.4.352.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kaplan S. 1995. The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework. J Environ Psychol. 15:169182. https://doi.org/10.1016/0272-4944(95)90001-2.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kaplan S, Berman MG. 2010. Directed attention as a common resource for executive functioning and self-regulation. Perspect Psychol Sci. 5:4357. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691609356784.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Karakaş S. 2020. A review of theta oscillation and its functional correlates. Int J Psychophysiol. 157:8299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.04.008.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kim SO, Jeong JE, Oh YA, Kim HR, Park SA. 2021. Comparing concentration levels and emotional states of children using electroencephalography during horticultural and nonhorticultural activities. HortScience. 56(3):324329. https://doi.org/10.21273/HORTSCI15522-20.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kim SO, Kim YJ, Park SA. 2022. Psychophysiological responses of adults according to cognitive demand levels for horticultural activities. Sustainability. 14(14):8252. https://doi.org/10.3390/su14148252.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kim SO, Oh YA, Park SA. 2020. Foliage plants improve concentration and emotional condition of elementary school students performing an intensive assignment. HortScience. 55:378385. https://doi.org/10.21273/HORTSCI14757-19.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Klem GH. 1999. The ten-twenty electrode system of the international federation. The international federation of clinical neurophysiology. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Suppl. 52:36. https://cir.nii.ac.jp/crid/1574231874767178624.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Klink K, Paßmann S, Kasten FH, Peter J. 2020. The modulation of cognitive performance with transcranial alternating current stimulation: A systematic review of frequency-specific effects. Brain Sci. 10:932. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10120932.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lee AY, Kim SO, Park SA. 2021. Attention and emotional states during horticultural activities of adults in 20s using electroencephalography: A pilot study. Sustainability. 13:12968. https://doi.org/10.3390/su132312968.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lee AY, Park SA, Kim JJ, So JM, Son KC. 2016. Kinematic and kinetic analysis of upper limb motions during horticultural activities. Hortic Sci Technol. 34(6):940958. https://doi.org/10.12972/kjhst.20160097.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lee AY, Park SA, Moon YJ, Son KC. 2018. Kinematic and kinetic analysis of horticultural activities for postural control and balance training. HortScience. 53(10):15411552. https://doi.org/10.21273/HORTSCI13361-1.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lina EI, Karwowski W. 2020. Applications of EEG indices for the quantification of human cognitive performance: A systematic review and bibliometric analysis. PLoS One. 15:e0242857. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242857.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Maddirala AK, Veluvolu KC. 2021. Eye-blink artifact removal from single channel EEG with k-means and SSA. Sci Rep. 11:11043. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-90437-7.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mazher M, Aziz AA, Malik AS, Amin HU. 2017. An EEG-based cognitive load assessment in multimedia learning using feature extraction and partial directed coherence. IEEE Access. 5:1481914829. https://doi.org/10.1109/ACCESS.2017.2731784.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Neuper C, Pfurtscheller G. 2001. Event-related dynamics of cortical rhythms: Frequency-specific features and functional correlates. Int J Psychophysiol. 43:4158. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-8760(01)00178-7.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Niedermeyer E, da Silva FL (eds). 2005. Electroencephalography: Basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields (5th ed). Raven Press, New York, NY, USA.

  • Oh YA, Kim SO, Park SA. 2019. Real foliage plants as visual stimuli to improve concentration and attention in elementary students. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 16(5):796. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16050796.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ohly H, White MP, Wheeler BW, Bethel A, Ukoumunne OC, Nikolaou V, Garside R. 2016. Attention restoration theory: A systematic review of the attention restoration potential of exposure to natural environments. J Toxicol Environ Health B. 19:305343. https://doi.org/10.1080/10937404.2016.1196155.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Osgood CE, Suci GJ, Tannenbaum P. 1957. The measurement of meaning. University of Illinois Press, Urbana, IL, USA.

  • Paas F, Tuovinen JE, Tabbers H, van Gerven PWM. 2003. Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance cognitive load theory. Educ Psychol. 38:6371. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326985EP3801_8.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Park BW. Introduction to neurofeedback. 2005. Korea Mental Health Institute, Seoul, Korea.

  • Park KJ, Jeong H. 2014. Assessing methods of heart rate variability. Korean J Clin Neurophysiol. 16:4954. https://doi.org/10.14253/kjcn.2014.16.2.49.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Park SA, Lee AY, Lee GJ, Kim DS, Kim WS, Shoemaker CA, Son KC. 2016. Horticultural activity interventions and outcomes. Hortic Sci Technol. 34:513527. https://doi.org/10.12972/kjhst.20160053.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Park SA, Lee AY, Lee KS, Son KC. 2014. Gardening tasks performed by adults are moderate- to high-intensity physical activities. HortTechnology. 24(1):5863. https://doi.org/10.21273/HORTTECH.24.1.58.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Park SA, Lee AY, Park HG, Son KC, Kim DS, Lee WR. 2017. Gardening intervention as a low- to moderate-intensity physical activity for improving blood lipid profiles, blood pressure, inflammation, and oxidative stress in women over the age of 70: A pilot study. HortScience. 52:200205. https://doi.org/10.21273/HORTSCI11232-16.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Park SA, Lee HS, Lee KS, Son KC, Shoemaker CA, 2013. The metabolic costs of gardening tasks in children. HortTechnology. 23(5):589594. https://doi.org/10.21273/HORTTECH.23.5.589.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Park SA, Lee KS, Son KC. 2011. Determining exercise intensities of gardening tasks as a physical activity using metabolic equivalents in older adults. HortScience. 46(12):17061710. https://doi.org/10.21273/HORTSCI.46.12.1706.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Park SA, Son SY, Lee AY, Park HG, Lee WL, Lee CH. 2020. Metabolite profiling revealed that a gardening activity program improves cognitive ability correlated with BDNF levels and serotonin metabolism in the elderly. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 17:541. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17020541.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pham T, Lau ZJ, Chen SHA, Makowski D. 2021. Heart rate variability in psychology: A review of HRV indices and an analysis tutorial. Sensors (Basel). 21:3998. https://doi.org/10.3390/s21123998.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pope RA, Thompson PJ, Rantell K, Stretton J, Wright MA, Foong J. 2019. Frontal lobe dysfunction as a predictor of depression and anxiety following temporal lobe epilepsy surgery. Epilepsy Res. 152:5966. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-25375-z.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rebsamen B, Kwok K, Penney TB. 2011. Evaluation of cognitive workload from EEG during a mental arithmetic task. Proc Human Factor Ergonom Society Annual Mtg. 55(1):13421345. https://doi.org/10.1177/1071181311551279.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shaffer F, Ginsberg JP. 2017. An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Front Public Health. 258. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Siddiqui SV, Chatterjee U, Kumar D, Siddiqui A, Goyal N. 2008. Neuropsychology of prefrontal cortex. Indian J Psychiatry. 50:202208. https://doi.org/10.4103/0019-5545.43634.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sowndhararajan K, Cho H, Yu B, Kim S. 2015. Effect of olfactory stimulation of isomeric aroma compounds, (+)-limonene and terpinolene on human electroencephalographic activity. Eur J Integr Med. 7:561566. https://doi.org/10.3390/scipharm84040724.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Staufenbiel SM, Brouwer AM, Keizer AW, van Wouwe NC. 2014. Effect of beta and gamma neurofeedback on memory and intelligence in the elderly. Biol Psychol. 95:7485. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.05.020.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stevens R, Galloway T, Halpin D, Willemsen-Dunlap A. 2016. Healthcare teams neurodynamically reorganize when resolving uncertainty. Entropy (Basel). 18:427. https://doi.org/10.3390/e18120427.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stilgoe JR. 2001. Gone barefoot lately? Am J Prev Med. 20:243244. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-3797(00)00319-6.

  • Sweller J. 1994. Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design. Learn Instr. 4:295312. https://doi.org/10.1016/0959-4752(94)90003-5.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sweller J. 2011. Cognitive load theory. Psychol Learn Motiv Cogn Educ. 55:3776. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-387691-1.00002-8.

  • Wang XJ. 2010. Neurophysiological and computational principles of cortical rhythms in cognition. Physiol Rev. 90:11951268. https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00035.2008.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Whear R, Coon JT, Bethel A, Abbott R, Stein K, Garside R. 2014. What is the impact of using outdoor spaces such as gardens on the physical and mental well-being of those with dementia? A systematic review of quantitative and qualitative evidence. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 15:697705. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jamda.2014.05.013.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wu YT, Lee AY, Choi NY, Park SA. 2022. Psychophysiological responses of cut flower fragrances as an olfactory stimulation by measurement of electroencephalogram in adults. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 19(18):11639. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191811639.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zarjam P, Epps J, Chen F. 2011. Spectral EEG featuresfor evaluating cognitive load. Presented at the Proceedings of the 2011 Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. Boston, MA, USA, 30 Aug–3 Sep 2011. https://doi.org/10.1109/IEMBS.2011.6090954.

  • Fig. 1.

    Experimental space layout. (A) Indoor experimental space. (B) Outdoor experimental space.

  • Fig. 2.

    Study procedure at each visit. EEG = electroencephalogram or electroencephalographic; ECG = electrocardiogram or electrocardiographic.

  • Fig. 3.

    International electrode arrangement (Stevens et al. 2016). (A) The channels in bold type, shaded in blue (Fp1 and Fp2) were measured in this study. (B) Electrode placement in electrocardiography. G = ground electrode; M = measuring electrode.

  • Antonenko P, Paas F, Grabner R, Van Gog T. 2010. Using electroencephalography to measure cognitive load. Educ Psychol Rev. 22:425438. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-010-9130-y.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Backs RW, Walrath LC. 1992. Eye movement and pupillary response indices of mental workload during visual search of symbolic displays. Appl Ergon. 23(4):243254. https://doi.org/10.1016/0003-6870(92)90152-L.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chandler P, Sweller J. 1991. Cognitive load theory and the format of instruction. Cogn Instr. 8:293332. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci0804_2.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chayer C, Freedman M. 2001. Frontal lobe functions. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 1:547552. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11910-001-0060-4.

  • Chen HM, Tu HM, Ho CI. 2013. Understanding biophilia leisure as facilitating well-being and the environment: An examination of participants’ attitudes toward horticultural activity. Leis Sci. 35:301319. https://doi.org/10.1080/01490400.2013.797323.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chiesura A. 2004. The role of urban parks for the sustainable city. Landsc Urban Plan. 68:129138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.landurbplan.2003.08.003.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Faller J, Cummings J, Saproo S, Sajda P. 2019. Regulation of arousal via online neurofeedback improves human performance in a demanding sensory-motor task. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 116:64826490. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1817207116.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fries P. 2015. Rhythms for cognition: Communication through coherence. Neuron. 88:220235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.034.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fuentes-Garcia JP, Pereira T, Castro MA, Carvalho Santos A, Villafaina S. 2019. Psychophysiological stress response of adolescent chess players during problem-solving tasks. Physiol Behav. 209:112609. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112609.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gale A, Edwards J. 1983. The EEG and human behavior. Physiological correlates of human behavior, p 99–127.

  • García-Monge A, Rodríguez-Navarro H, González-Calvo G, Bores-García D. 2020. Brain activity during different throwing games: EEG exploratory study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 17:6796. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17186796.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gidlow CJ, Jones MV, Hurst G, Masterson D, Clark-Carter D, Tarvainen MP, Smith G, Nieuwenhuijsen M. 2016. Where to put your best foot forward: Psycho-physiological responses to walking in natural and urban environments. J Environ Psychol. 45:2229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.11.003.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Heckman MA, Weil J, de Mejia EG. 2010. Caffeine (1,3,7-trimethylxanthine) in foods: A comprehensive review on consumption, functionality, safety, and regulatory matters. J Food Sci. 75:R77R87. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-3841.2010.01561.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Herrmann CS, Munk MH, Engel AK. 2004. Cognitive functions of gamma-band activity: Memory match and utilization. Trends Cogn Sci. 8(8):347355. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.06.006.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hung SH, Chang CY. 2021. Health benefits of evidence-based biophilic-designed environments: A review. J People Plants Environ. 24:116. https://doi.org/10.11628/ksppe.2021.24.1.1.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ives CD, Abson DJ, Wehrden HV, Dorninger C, Klaniecki K, Fischer J. 2018. Reconnecting with nature for sustainability. Sustain Sci. 13:13891397. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11625-018-0542-9.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jasper HH. 1958. The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 10:371375.

  • Kabisch N, Püffel C, Masztalerz O, Hemmerling J, Kraemer R. 2021. Physiological and psychological effects of visits to different urban green and street environments in older people: A field experiment in a dense inner-city area. Landsc Urban Plan. 207:103998. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.landurbplan.2020.103998.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kang KJ, Kang MJ. 2021. Effects of horticultural therapy on Korean elderly with dementia: A meta-analysis. J Korean Acad Psychiatr Ment Health Nurs. 30(4):352368. https://doi.org/10.12934/jkpmhn.2021.30.4.352.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kaplan S. 1995. The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework. J Environ Psychol. 15:169182. https://doi.org/10.1016/0272-4944(95)90001-2.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kaplan S, Berman MG. 2010. Directed attention as a common resource for executive functioning and self-regulation. Perspect Psychol Sci. 5:4357. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691609356784.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Karakaş S. 2020. A review of theta oscillation and its functional correlates. Int J Psychophysiol. 157:8299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.04.008.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kim SO, Jeong JE, Oh YA, Kim HR, Park SA. 2021. Comparing concentration levels and emotional states of children using electroencephalography during horticultural and nonhorticultural activities. HortScience. 56(3):324329. https://doi.org/10.21273/HORTSCI15522-20.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kim SO, Kim YJ, Park SA. 2022. Psychophysiological responses of adults according to cognitive demand levels for horticultural activities. Sustainability. 14(14):8252. https://doi.org/10.3390/su14148252.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kim SO, Oh YA, Park SA. 2020. Foliage plants improve concentration and emotional condition of elementary school students performing an intensive assignment. HortScience. 55:378385. https://doi.org/10.21273/HORTSCI14757-19.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Klem GH. 1999. The ten-twenty electrode system of the international federation. The international federation of clinical neurophysiology. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Suppl. 52:36. https://cir.nii.ac.jp/crid/1574231874767178624.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Klink K, Paßmann S, Kasten FH, Peter J. 2020. The modulation of cognitive performance with transcranial alternating current stimulation: A systematic review of frequency-specific effects. Brain Sci. 10:932. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10120932.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lee AY, Kim SO, Park SA. 2021. Attention and emotional states during horticultural activities of adults in 20s using electroencephalography: A pilot study. Sustainability. 13:12968. https://doi.org/10.3390/su132312968.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lee AY, Park SA, Kim JJ, So JM, Son KC. 2016. Kinematic and kinetic analysis of upper limb motions during horticultural activities. Hortic Sci Technol. 34(6):940958. https://doi.org/10.12972/kjhst.20160097.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lee AY, Park SA, Moon YJ, Son KC. 2018. Kinematic and kinetic analysis of horticultural activities for postural control and balance training. HortScience. 53(10):15411552. https://doi.org/10.21273/HORTSCI13361-1.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lina EI, Karwowski W. 2020. Applications of EEG indices for the quantification of human cognitive performance: A systematic review and bibliometric analysis. PLoS One. 15:e0242857. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242857.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Maddirala AK, Veluvolu KC. 2021. Eye-blink artifact removal from single channel EEG with k-means and SSA. Sci Rep. 11:11043. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-90437-7.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mazher M, Aziz AA, Malik AS, Amin HU. 2017. An EEG-based cognitive load assessment in multimedia learning using feature extraction and partial directed coherence. IEEE Access. 5:1481914829. https://doi.org/10.1109/ACCESS.2017.2731784.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Neuper C, Pfurtscheller G. 2001. Event-related dynamics of cortical rhythms: Frequency-specific features and functional correlates. Int J Psychophysiol. 43:4158. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-8760(01)00178-7.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Niedermeyer E, da Silva FL (eds). 2005. Electroencephalography: Basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields (5th ed). Raven Press, New York, NY, USA.

  • Oh YA, Kim SO, Park SA. 2019. Real foliage plants as visual stimuli to improve concentration and attention in elementary students. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 16(5):796. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16050796.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ohly H, White MP, Wheeler BW, Bethel A, Ukoumunne OC, Nikolaou V, Garside R. 2016. Attention restoration theory: A systematic review of the attention restoration potential of exposure to natural environments. J Toxicol Environ Health B. 19:305343. https://doi.org/10.1080/10937404.2016.1196155.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Osgood CE, Suci GJ, Tannenbaum P. 1957. The measurement of meaning. University of Illinois Press, Urbana, IL, USA.

  • Paas F, Tuovinen JE, Tabbers H, van Gerven PWM. 2003. Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance cognitive load theory. Educ Psychol. 38:6371. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326985EP3801_8.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Park BW. Introduction to neurofeedback. 2005. Korea Mental Health Institute, Seoul, Korea.

  • Park KJ, Jeong H. 2014. Assessing methods of heart rate variability. Korean J Clin Neurophysiol. 16:4954. https://doi.org/10.14253/kjcn.2014.16.2.49.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Park SA, Lee AY, Lee GJ, Kim DS, Kim WS, Shoemaker CA, Son KC. 2016. Horticultural activity interventions and outcomes. Hortic Sci Technol. 34:513527. https://doi.org/10.12972/kjhst.20160053.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Park SA, Lee AY, Lee KS, Son KC. 2014. Gardening tasks performed by adults are moderate- to high-intensity physical activities. HortTechnology. 24(1):5863. https://doi.org/10.21273/HORTTECH.24.1.58.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Park SA, Lee AY, Park HG, Son KC, Kim DS, Lee WR. 2017. Gardening intervention as a low- to moderate-intensity physical activity for improving blood lipid profiles, blood pressure, inflammation, and oxidative stress in women over the age of 70: A pilot study. HortScience. 52:200205. https://doi.org/10.21273/HORTSCI11232-16.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Park SA, Lee HS, Lee KS, Son KC, Shoemaker CA, 2013. The metabolic costs of gardening tasks in children. HortTechnology. 23(5):589594. https://doi.org/10.21273/HORTTECH.23.5.589.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Park SA, Lee KS, Son KC. 2011. Determining exercise intensities of gardening tasks as a physical activity using metabolic equivalents in older adults. HortScience. 46(12):17061710. https://doi.org/10.21273/HORTSCI.46.12.1706.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Park SA, Son SY, Lee AY, Park HG, Lee WL, Lee CH. 2020. Metabolite profiling revealed that a gardening activity program improves cognitive ability correlated with BDNF levels and serotonin metabolism in the elderly. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 17:541. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17020541.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pham T, Lau ZJ, Chen SHA, Makowski D. 2021. Heart rate variability in psychology: A review of HRV indices and an analysis tutorial. Sensors (Basel). 21:3998. https://doi.org/10.3390/s21123998.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pope RA, Thompson PJ, Rantell K, Stretton J, Wright MA, Foong J. 2019. Frontal lobe dysfunction as a predictor of depression and anxiety following temporal lobe epilepsy surgery. Epilepsy Res. 152:5966. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-25375-z.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rebsamen B, Kwok K, Penney TB. 2011. Evaluation of cognitive workload from EEG during a mental arithmetic task. Proc Human Factor Ergonom Society Annual Mtg. 55(1):13421345. https://doi.org/10.1177/1071181311551279.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shaffer F, Ginsberg JP. 2017. An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Front Public Health. 258. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Siddiqui SV, Chatterjee U, Kumar D, Siddiqui A, Goyal N. 2008. Neuropsychology of prefrontal cortex. Indian J Psychiatry. 50:202208. https://doi.org/10.4103/0019-5545.43634.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sowndhararajan K, Cho H, Yu B, Kim S. 2015. Effect of olfactory stimulation of isomeric aroma compounds, (+)-limonene and terpinolene on human electroencephalographic activity. Eur J Integr Med. 7:561566. https://doi.org/10.3390/scipharm84040724.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Staufenbiel SM, Brouwer AM, Keizer AW, van Wouwe NC. 2014. Effect of beta and gamma neurofeedback on memory and intelligence in the elderly. Biol Psychol. 95:7485. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.05.020.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stevens R, Galloway T, Halpin D, Willemsen-Dunlap A. 2016. Healthcare teams neurodynamically reorganize when resolving uncertainty. Entropy (Basel). 18:427. https://doi.org/10.3390/e18120427.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stilgoe JR. 2001. Gone barefoot lately? Am J Prev Med. 20:243244. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-3797(00)00319-6.

  • Sweller J. 1994. Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design. Learn Instr. 4:295312. https://doi.org/10.1016/0959-4752(94)90003-5.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sweller J. 2011. Cognitive load theory. Psychol Learn Motiv Cogn Educ. 55:3776. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-387691-1.00002-8.

  • Wang XJ. 2010. Neurophysiological and computational principles of cortical rhythms in cognition. Physiol Rev. 90:11951268. https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00035.2008.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Whear R, Coon JT, Bethel A, Abbott R, Stein K, Garside R. 2014. What is the impact of using outdoor spaces such as gardens on the physical and mental well-being of those with dementia? A systematic review of quantitative and qualitative evidence. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 15:697705. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jamda.2014.05.013.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wu YT, Lee AY, Choi NY, Park SA. 2022. Psychophysiological responses of cut flower fragrances as an olfactory stimulation by measurement of electroencephalogram in adults. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 19(18):11639. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191811639.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zarjam P, Epps J, Chen F. 2011. Spectral EEG featuresfor evaluating cognitive load. Presented at the Proceedings of the 2011 Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. Boston, MA, USA, 30 Aug–3 Sep 2011. https://doi.org/10.1109/IEMBS.2011.6090954.

Yun-Jin Kim Department of Bio and Healing Convergence, Graduate School, Konkuk University, Seoul, 05029, Republic of Korea

Search for other papers by Yun-Jin Kim in
Google Scholar
Close
and
Sin-Ae Park Department of Bio and Healing Convergence, Graduate School, Konkuk University, Seoul 05029, Republic of Korea, and Department of Systems Biotechnology, Konkuk Institute of Technology, Konkuk University, Seoul 05029, Republic of Korea

Search for other papers by Sin-Ae Park in
Google Scholar
Close

Contributor Notes

This work was supported by the KU Research Professor Program of Konkuk University and was carried out with the support of the Cooperative Research Program for Agriculture Science and Technology Development (Project No. RS-2021-RD009877), Rural Development Administration, Republic of Korea.

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

S.-A.P. is the corresponding author. E-mail: sapark42@konkuk.ac.kr.

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 0 0 0
Full Text Views 387 387 13
PDF Downloads 238 238 21
  • Fig. 1.

    Experimental space layout. (A) Indoor experimental space. (B) Outdoor experimental space.

  • Fig. 2.

    Study procedure at each visit. EEG = electroencephalogram or electroencephalographic; ECG = electrocardiogram or electrocardiographic.

  • Fig. 3.

    International electrode arrangement (Stevens et al. 2016). (A) The channels in bold type, shaded in blue (Fp1 and Fp2) were measured in this study. (B) Electrode placement in electrocardiography. G = ground electrode; M = measuring electrode.

Advertisement
PP Systems Measuring Far Red Advert

 

Advertisement
Save